Grandma's Experiences Leave a Mark on Your Genes
By Dan Hurley
Your ancestors' lousy childhoods or excellent adventures
might change your personality, bequeathing anxiety or resilience by altering
the epigenetic expressions of genes in the brain.
Darwin and Freud walk into a bar. Two alcoholic mice — a
mother and her son — sit on two bar stools, lapping gin from two thimbles.
The mother mouse looks up and says, “Hey, geniuses, tell me
how my son got into this sorry state.”
“Bad inheritance,”
says Darwin.
“Bad mothering,” says
Freud.
For over a hundred years, those two views — nature or
nurture, biology or psychology — offered opposing explanations for how
behaviors develop and persist, not only within a single individual but across
generations.
And then, in 1992, two young scientists following in Freud’s
and Darwin’s footsteps actually did walk into a bar. And by the time they
walked out, a few beers later, they had begun to forge a revolutionary new
synthesis of how life experiences could directly affect your genes — and not
only your own life experiences, but those of your mother’s, grandmother’s and
beyond.
The bar was in Madrid, where the Cajal Institute, Spain’s
oldest academic center for the study of neurobiology, was holding an
international meeting. Moshe Szyf, a molecular biologist and geneticist at
McGill University in Montreal, had never studied psychology or neurology, but
he had been talked into attending by a colleague who thought his work might
have some application. Likewise, Michael Meaney, a McGill neurobiologist, had
been talked into attending by the same colleague, who thought Meaney’s research
into animal models of maternal neglect might benefit from Szyf’s perspective.
“I can still
visualize the place — it was a corner bar that specialized in pizza,” Meaney
says. “Moshe, being kosher, was interested in kosher calories. Beer is kosher.
Moshe can drink beer anywhere. And I’m Irish. So it was perfect.”
The two engaged in animated conversation about a hot new
line of research in genetics. Since the 1970s, researchers had known that the
tightly wound spools of DNA inside each cell’s nucleus require something extra
to tell them exactly which genes to transcribe, whether for a heart cell, a
liver cell or a brain cell.
One such extra element is the methyl group, a common
structural component of organic molecules. The methyl group works like a
placeholder in a cookbook, attaching to the DNA within each cell to select only
those recipes — er, genes — necessary for that particular cell’s proteins.
Because methyl groups are attached to the genes, residing beside but separate
from the double-helix DNA code, the field was dubbed epigenetics, from the
prefix epi (Greek for over, outer, above).
Originally these epigenetic changes were believed to occur
only during fetal development. But pioneering studies showed that molecular
bric-a-brac could be added to DNA in adulthood, setting off a cascade of
cellular changes resulting in cancer. Sometimes methyl groups attached to DNA
thanks to changes in diet; other times, exposure to certain chemicals appeared
to be the cause. Szyf showed that correcting epigenetic changes with drugs
could cure certain cancers in animals.
Geneticists were especially surprised to find that
epigenetic change could be passed down from parent to child, one generation
after the next. A study from Randy Jirtle of Duke University showed that when
female mice are fed a diet rich in methyl groups, the fur pigment of subsequent
offspring is permanently altered. Without any change to DNA at all, methyl
groups could be added or subtracted, and the changes were inherited much like a
mutation in a gene.
Now, at the bar in Madrid, Szyf and Meaney considered a
hypothesis as improbable as it was profound: If diet and chemicals can cause
epigenetic changes, could certain experiences — child neglect, drug abuse or
other severe stresses — also set off epigenetic changes to the DNA inside the
neurons of a person’s brain? That question turned out to be the basis of a new
field, behavioral epigenetics, now so vibrant it has spawned dozens of studies
and suggested profound new treatments to heal the brain.
According to the new insights of behavioral epigenetics,
traumatic experiences in our past, or in our recent ancestors’ past, leave
molecular scars adhering to our DNA. Jews whose great-grandparents were chased
from their Russian shtetls; Chinese whose grandparents lived through the
ravages of the Cultural Revolution; young immigrants from Africa whose parents
survived massacres; adults of every ethnicity who grew up with alcoholic or
abusive parents — all carry with them more than just memories.
Like silt deposited on the cogs of a finely tuned machine
after the seawater of a tsunami recedes, our experiences, and those of our
forebears, are never gone, even if they have been forgotten. They become a part
of us, a molecular residue holding fast to our genetic scaffolding. The DNA
remains the same, but psychological and behavioral tendencies are inherited.
You might have inherited not just your grandmother’s knobby knees, but also her
predisposition toward depression caused by the neglect she suffered as a newborn.
Or not. If your grandmother was adopted by nurturing
parents, you might be enjoying the boost she received thanks to their love and
support. The mechanisms of behavioral epigenetics underlie not only deficits
and weaknesses but strengths and resiliencies, too. And for those unlucky
enough to descend from miserable or withholding grandparents, emerging drug
treatments could reset not just mood, but the epigenetic changes themselves.
Like grandmother’s vintage dress, you could wear it or have it altered. The
genome has long been known as the blueprint of life, but the epigenome is
life’s Etch A Sketch: Shake it hard enough, and you can wipe clean the family
curse.
Twenty years after helping to set off a revolution, Meaney
sits behind a wide walnut table that serves as his desk. A January storm has
deposited half a foot of snow outside the picture windows lining his
fourth-floor corner office at the Douglas Institute, a mental health affiliate
of McGill. He has the rugged good looks and tousled salt-and-pepper hair of
someone found on a ski slope — precisely where he plans to go this weekend. On
the floor lies an arrangement of helium balloons in various stages of deflation.
“Happy 60th!” one announces.
“I’ve always been
interested in what makes people different from each other,” he says. “The way
we act, the way we behave — some people are optimistic, some are pessimistic.
What produces that variation? Evolution selects the variance that is most
successful, but what produces the grist for the mill?”
Meaney pursued the question of individual differences by
studying how the rearing habits of mother rats caused lifelong changes in their
offspring. Research dating back to the 1950s had shown that rats handled by
humans for as little as five to 15 minutes per day during their first three
weeks of life grew up to be calmer and less reactive to stressful environments
compared with their non-handled littermates. Seeking to tease out the mechanism
behind such an enduring effect, Meaney and others established that the benefit
was not actually conveyed by the human handling. Rather, the handling simply
provoked the rats’ mothers to lick and groom their pups more, and to engage more
often in a behavior called arched-back nursing, in which the mother gives the
pups extra room to suckle against her underside.
“It’s all about the
tactile stimulation,” Meaney says.
In a landmark 1997 paper in Science, he showed that natural
variations in the amount of licking and grooming received during infancy had a
direct effect on how stress hormones, including corticosterone, were expressed
in adulthood. The more licking as babies, the lower the stress hormones as
grown-ups. It was almost as if the mother rats were licking away at a genetic
dimmer switch. What the paper didn’t explain was how such a thing could be
possible.
"What we had done up to that point in time was to
identify maternal care and its influence on specific genes,” Meaney says. “But
epigenetics wasn’t a topic I knew very much about.”
And then he met Szyf.
“I was going to be a dentist,” Szyf says with a laugh.
Slight, pale and balding, he sits in a small office at the back of his bustling
laboratory — a room so Spartan, it contains just a single picture, a photograph
of two embryos in a womb.
Needing to write a thesis in the late 1970s for his
doctorate in dentistry at Hebrew University of Jerusalem, Szyf approached a young
biochemistry professor named Aharon Razin, who had recently made a splash by
publishing his first few studies in some of the world’s top scientific
journals. The studies were the first to show that the action of genes could be
modulated by structures called methyl groups, a subject about which Szyf knew
precisely nothing. But he needed a thesis adviser, and Razin was there. Szyf
found himself swept up to the forefront of the hot new field of epigenetics and
never looked back.
Until researchers like Razin came along, the basic story
line on how genes get transcribed in a cell was neat and simple. DNA is the
master code, residing inside the nucleus of every cell; RNA transcribes the
code to build whatever proteins the cell needs. Then some of Razin’s colleagues
showed that methyl groups could attach to cytosine, one of the chemical bases
in DNA and RNA.
It was Razin, working with fellow biochemist Howard Cedar,
who showed these attachments weren’t just brief, meaningless affairs. The
methyl groups could become married permanently to the DNA, getting replicated
right along with it through a hundred generations. As in any good marriage,
moreover, the attachment of the methyl groups significantly altered the
behavior of whichever gene they wed, inhibiting its transcription, much like a
jealous spouse. It did so, Razin and Cedar showed, by tightening the thread of
DNA as it wrapped around a molecular spool, called a histone, inside the
nucleus. The tighter it is wrapped, the harder to produce proteins from the
gene.
Consider what that means: Without a mutation to the DNA code
itself, the attached methyl groups cause long-term, heritable change in gene
function. Other molecules, called acetyl groups, were found to play the
opposite role, unwinding DNA around the histone spool, and so making it easier
for RNA to transcribe a given gene.
Read more @ nexusilluminati.blogspot
No comments:
Post a Comment
Please leave a comment.